Correlation between motor and phosphene thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study
r Human Brain Mapping 29:662–670 (2008) r Correlation Between Motor and Phosphene Thresholds: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study Choi Deblieck,1 Benjamin Thompson,2 Marco Iacoboni,1,3,4 and Allan D. Wu1,5*
1Ahmanson-Lovelace Brain Mapping Center, UCLA, Los Angeles, California
2Department of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, California
3Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA,
4Brain Research Institute, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, California
5Department of Neurology, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, California
Abstract: Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has become a common tool for the brain mapping of awide variety of cognitive functions. Because TMS over cortical regions of interest other than motor cortexoften does not produce easily observable effects, the ability to calibrate TMS intensity for stimulation overnonmotor regions can be problematic. Previous studies reported no correlation between motor thresholds(MT) over the motor cortex and phosphene thresholds (PT) over the visual cortex. However, differentthresholding methods, lighting, and eye-closure conditions were used to determine MT and PT. We investi-gated the correlation between resting MT (rMT), active MT (aMT), and PT in 27 dark-adapted healthy vol-unteers. All thresholds were measured with eyes-open in the dark and determined by gradually reducingstimulation intensity downward. All subjects had aMT and rMT; 21 subjects had measurable PT. rMT was70.4% 6 9.8% (mean 6 SD of maximum stimulator output); aMT was 61.1% 6 7.9%; PT was 82.2% 610.1%. A significant positive correlation was found between aMT and PT (r ¼ 0.53; P ¼ 0.014) with a trendtoward correlation between rMT and PT (r ¼ 0.43; P ¼ 0.052). Our results suggest that sensitivity to TMSover visual and motor cortices may be correlated under similar thresholding procedures. They also providea rationale for the use of easily obtained aMT to calibrate TMS intensities in brain mapping studies thatemploy TMS in cortical regions besides motor cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 29:662–670, 2008.
C 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Key words: motor threshold (MT); phosphene threshold (PT); cortical excitability; transcranial magnetic
Contract grant sponsor: NINDS, NIH; Contract grant number:
*Correspondence to: Allan D. Wu, Department of Neurology,
K23-NS045764; Contract grant sponsor: NCRR, NIH; Contract
David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, 710 Westwood Plaza,
grant numbers: RR12169, RR13642, RR00865; For generous sup-
Reed Bldg A-153, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1769.
port, we thank: Brain Mapping Medical Research Organization,
Brain Mapping Support Foundation, Pierson-Lovelace Foundation,
Received for publication 29 January 2007; Revised 23 April 2007;
The Ahmanson Foundation, William M. and Linda R. Dietel Phi-
lanthropic Fund at the Northern Piedmont Community Founda-
tion, Tamkin Foundation, Jennifer Jones-Simon Foundation, Capi-
Published online 27 June 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.
tal Group Companies Charitable Foundation, Robson Family and
C 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
r Correlation Between Motor and Phosphene Thresholds r INTRODUCTION
potentially increases the variance of TMS effects, reducesstatistical power of TMS studies, and represents a major li-
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a noninva-
mitation when considering applications of TMS as a brain
sive method for brain stimulation that has become an im-
mapping tool across multiple cortical regions [Robertson
portant modality for mapping brain–behavior relationships
et al., 2003]. The assumption that a relevant proportion of
in cognitive neuroscience [Robertson et al., 2003]. In con-
TMS threshold measures between different neocortical
trast to correlational neuroimaging methods, such as fMRI
regions could reflect a shared component of within-indi-
or PET, TMS interacts with ongoing brain activity around
vidual responsiveness to TMS has been criticized by previ-
the region of cortex where induced current under the coil
ous studies that demonstrated no correlation between MT
is produced. Consequently, TMS can be used to directly
and PT [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig
evaluate the critical and causal significance of the stimu-
et al., 2003; Stewart et al., 2001b].
lated areas. In conjunction with behavioral tasks, TMS
However, none of those studies used similar methods to
studies can directly demonstrate causal relationships
determine both thresholds (see Table I). For example, while
between brain areas and tasks. For example, TMS over the
PT is usually measured under dark-adapted conditions, MT
occipital lobe can disrupt Braille reading in congenitally
is not. The approach toward threshold (upwards, down-
blind individuals [Cohen et al., 1997], TMS over Broca’s
wards, or variable) is not consistent between or within stud-
area can interfere with imitation of hand postures [Heiser
ies [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig et al.,
et al., 2003], and TMS over the posterior parietal cortex can
2003; Stewart et al., 2001b]. Furthermore, other factors may
disrupt feedforward error correction in visually guided
have led to additional confounds in previous studies. For
example, paired pulses of TMS, which enhance phosphene
TMS effects depend on factors, such as cortical target,
perception, have been used over the visual cortex in contrast
TMS coil geometry, pulse waveform, and stimulation pa-
to single TMS pulses over the motor cortex [Antal et al.,
rameters, such as intensity, frequency, and number of
2003b; Boroojerdi et al. 2002] or different machines were
pulses. While the nature of the brain mapping experiment
used for PT and MT determinations [Antal et al., 2003b].
can determine cortical targets of interest, the choice of stimu-
Since MTs can be affected by these factors [Leon-Sarmiento
lation parameters is not always straightforward. Even if all
et al., 2005; Tranulis et al., 2006], the possibility exists that
technical factors that determine the topography and strength
these different methodological factors could have limited
of the magnetic field are identical, individual differences in
the detection of a correlation between MT and PT.
the intrinsic responsiveness, or excitability, of each subject’s
The aim of the present study was to investigate the rela-
brain to stimulation will introduce unwanted variability in
tionship between MT and PT with psychophysically simi-
TMS effects [Robertson et al., 2003]. To assure comparability
lar thresholding procedures over motor and visual cortices.
between experimental conditions, TMS intensities should be
Specifically, we sought to determine all thresholds under
ideally calibrated such that TMS pulses produce a constant
both dark-adapted conditions and a uniform systematic
neurophysiological effect across subjects.
downward approach toward threshold with single TMS
In addition to being variable across subjects, excitability
pulses from the same TMS coil. A finding of a significant
also varies across different cortical regions and in different
MT and PT correlation would suggest some level of com-
contexts within a given subject [Robertson et al., 2003].
mon excitability across these two cortical regions. In addi-
Over the motor cortex, excitability can be quantified using
tion, a significant result would provide a rationale to the
a measurable motor evoked potential response (MEP) in a
practice of calibrating TMS intensities over different corti-
contralateral muscle. The motor threshold (MT) is com-
monly defined as the minimum TMS intensity that elicits aMEP above a minimal size and is routinely determined ineach individual subject prior to an experiment. Experimen-
SUBJECTS AND METHODS
tal stimulation intensities can then be set at a percentage
Subjects
of this MT, which assures that a suprathreshold TMS in-tensity used over the motor cortex in one subject will be
We recruited and obtained informed consent from 27
equivalently suprathreshold for another. Over the occipital
healthy subjects excluding those with a previous history of
cortex, excitability can be assessed using TMS-induced
neurological or psychiatric disorders, who did not take
phosphenes as a region-specific response measure. Analo-
any regular medications, and who did not have exclusions
gous to MT, a phosphene threshold (PT) can be defined as
relevant to TMS. Study procedures were approved by the
the minimum TMS intensity that elicits perception of phos-
UCLA Medical Institutional Review Board.
phenes. The PT then becomes a valid reference intensityfor TMS studies of visual perception [Boroojerdi et al.,
TMS Procedures
2002; Gothe et al., 2002; Hotson and Anand, 1999;Kammer, 1999; Stewart et al., 2001a].
All subjects were dark-adapted for the study by donning
The uncertainty in calibrating intensities across subjects
lightproof goggles at the start of the experimental session.
for brain regions beyond the motor and visual cortices
Goggles were adjusted to ensure that no light was visible.
r Deblieck et al. r
Goggles were designed not to produce pressure on eyelidsand preserve normal blinking. The room was darkened.
Participants were continually reminded to keep eyes open
and to fixate forward throughout each thresholding proce-
dure. Since different examiners may constitute a significant
source of variability [Chaudry et al., 1991], we had the
same two investigators present at every session who
supervised a consistent application of methods. Each in-vestigator conducted the same thresholding procedure
throughout this study. One investigator performed all
motor thresholding procedures. A second investigator was
only interested in measuring PT for a vision study that
required participants to see phosphenes, and performed allsubsequent phosphene thresholding procedures. The sec-
ond investigator was blinded to the purpose of this study
since no correlation was expected to be found. In addition,
the thresholding procedures were spread out over thecourse of several weeks, and no correlation analysis was
computed until all subjects for the vision study were
Thresholding procedures were conducted on the same
day in the following order: (1) measurement of resting
motor threshold (rMT), (2) active motor threshold (aMT),
and (3) phosphene threshold (PT). Goggles were worn for
at least 15 min before rMT thresholding procedures began. Significant
Adaptation to dark was present for 45 min by the time
PT procedures began. To ensure parallel procedures, all
thresholds were measured by consistently starting from a
clearly suprathreshold intensity and gradually reducing
stimulation intensity in steps of 1% until threshold inten-
Reporting
sity was obtained. No thresholds were approached from
below. Participants wore a tight lycra cap, on which grids
were drawn over the region of the left primary motor cor-
tex and left occipital visual areas.
We used a Magstim SuperRapid biphasic stimulator
with a figure-8 coil (14 cm width) for all motor and PT.
All following thresholds are expressed in percent of maxi-
mum stimulator output (MSO) (peak field strength 2 T). elational Motor Thresholding Procedures
For rMT and aMT, the figure-8 coil was held tangen-
tially to the skull and mediolaterally with the handle
pointing backwards and at a 458 angle from the sagittal
midline [Brasil-Neto et al., 1992]. Thus, the induced cur-
rent pointed forward in a roughly perpendicular manner
to the fictitious line of the central sulcus.
Surface EMG electrodes were placed over the right first
dorsal interosseus (FDI) muscle. EMG was sampled at 1
kHZ, amplified, and 1-1 kHz bandpass filtered. MEP sizes
were measured as peak-to-peak amplitudes. Aliasing of
higher frequency components in the EMG signal, where
power is minimal, is unlikely to affect thresholding results.
To measure rMT, single pulses of TMS were delivered
over the left motor cortex, while the right FDI was kept
relaxed. Trials where baseline EMG, in an interval 100 ms
prior to TMS pulse, showed visible EMG activity (>20 lV)
r Correlation Between Motor and Phosphene Thresholds r
were discarded. TMS pulses were delivered, while the coil
close to parameters that would induce reproducible phos-
was moved systematically, first at and then, between grid
phenes. Positive responses were then qualified as either
points. The location that evoked the largest and most reli-
central or peripheral in nature. The quality of the visual
able MEP amplitudes was designated the motor hotspot.
phenomena was further assessed with open questions (tex-
Starting suprathreshold intensities induced clearly dis-
tured, colored, shaped and so on). Candidate phosphenes
tinguishable MEP’s with every TMS pulse. Intensities were
were validated by moving the coil laterally to ensure that
then lowered by 1% decrements. The lowest intensity with
the perceived phosphene shifted location in a predictable
the coil at the motor hotspot for which peak-to-peak MEP
manner, validating the retinotopic nature of the visual per-
amplitudes greater than 50 lV occurred in at least 5 out of
cept. The location that evoked the brightest and most reli-
able phosphene was designated the visual hotspot. Then,
For aMT, subjects were asked to squeeze a small cylin-
from the suprathreshold intensity, intensities were lowered
der with a light steady pinch grip, while FDI activation
by 1% increments. The lowest stimulation intensity at
was monitored online, using EMG to ensure a constant av-
which stable phosphenes were perceived in at least 5 of 10
erage level of activity around 100 lV. With the coil held
stimulations was recorded as the PT.
over the same hotspot, TMS intensity was then lowered by
TMS-induced phosphene perception can be improved by
1% increments from rMT. The lowest intensity for which
training in individuals over time or by a period of dark
peak-to-peak MEPs greater than 100 lV above baseline
adaptation [Boroojerdi et al., 2000a]. Most studies using
EMG occurred in 5 out of 10 trials was designated the
phosphenes for calibration purposes have not typically
aMT. Throughout rMT and aMT procedures, subjects were
trained participants to see phosphenes before a threshold-
frequently asked to keep their eyes open, while looking
ing session, however our phosphene screening procedure
gave multiple opportunities for subjects to become familiarwith the concept and appearance of TMS induced phos-
Phosphene Thresholding Procedures
phenes. We dark-adapted subjects for 45 min, duringwhich we were able to record motor thresholds on all sub-
All PT procedures were done with the FDI muscles
jects, while increasing chances of reliable phosphene detec-
relaxed. To elicit phosphenes, the coil was positioned
tion. Also, because we approached PT by decreasing TMS
with the handle pointing upwards, parallel to the sub-
intensities, a phosphene was reported on every trial for the
ject’s spine [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002;
early part of thresholding procedures allowing participants
Stewart et al., 2001b]. The initial position of the coil was
to recognize a reliable precept of phosphenes before deter-
midline, 2 cm above the inion. Single pulses of TMS were
mining PT. This descending approach to phosphene
delivered over occipital cortex, while the coil was moved
threshold was selected to reduce the risk of participants
over a 1 3 1 cm2 interval grid marked on the lycra cap.
having artificially high thresholds as they were not waiting
The coil was moved systematically over the left visual
for a phosphene to appear, but rather for one that they
cortex to induce the perception of phosphenes in the right
visual field. All phosphene percepts were initiallydetermined at 100% MSO. The phosphene localization
Data Analysis
procedure was designed to maximize the likelihood thatreliable phosphenes were detected: during our first pass
rMT, aMT, and PT were compared using one-way
over the grid, participants were asked to attend to the
repeated measures ANOVA with least significant differ-
whole visual field and report the presence or absence of
ence post-hoc contrasts. Pearson correlation coefficients
any induced visual phenomena after each TMS pulse. If
were computed for each pair of threshold comparisons.
they did not report a reliable phosphene, each site on the
Significance was set at P < 0.05 to assess differences from
grid was tested three times at 100% MSO. After a 10-min-
break, the concept of a phosphene was explained again,and the whole procedure was repeated all over again. Af-
ter this screening procedure, if no phosphene wasreported, we felt it reasonable to stop.
Resting and aMT were measurable in all 27 participants.
Once a valid phosphene was reported, the coil was
rMT ranged between 51 and 87% MSO (mean ¼ 70.4; SD
moved until a bright, reliable phosphene was reported in a
¼ 9.8); aMT ranged between 47 and 77% MSO (mean ¼
paracentral location approximately within the central 88 of
61.1; SD ¼ 7.9). 21 out of 27 participants saw reproducible
the visual field. Phosphene location was reported by par-
phosphenes and had a measurable PT ranging from 59
ticipants either by indicating the approximate position of
and 99% MSO (mean ¼ 82.2; SD ¼ 10.1) over the mean tar-
the phosphene percept in visual space by pointing out in
get site of 1 cm lateral and 3 cm above the inion. In most
front of themselves or pointing on the front of the goggles,
participants, phosphenes were small, diffuse, white flashes
whichever they felt more comfortable doing. Uncertain
in the paracenter of the visual field that tended to grow
responses were classified as absent phosphenes, but often
smaller and dimmer with lower stimulator intensities.
suggested that either coil location and/or intensity was
Four participants perceived colored static phosphenes.
r Deblieck et al. r Figure 1.
(a) Relationship between phosphene thresholds (PT) and active motor thresholds (rMT). PT and aMT are significantly correlated (P ¼ 0.014). (b) Relationship between PT and resting motor thresholds (rMT) (P ¼ 0.052). All thresholds are in percent of maximum stimulator output.
Subjects that did not perceive phosphenes revealed a
2003], the present data suggest that aMT may help guide
slightly higher aMT and rMT (aMT 65.3 6 5.3 SD, rMT
stimulation intensity over the visual cortex, and perhaps
74.7 6 8.9) compared with subjects who did perceive phos-
over other nonmotor regions. More clearly, as our finding
phenes (aMT 59.9 6 8.2 SD; rMT 69.2 6 9.9), but the dif-
is at variance with other studies using different methodol-
ferences were not significant (aMT, P ¼ 0.14; rMT P ¼
ogies, these data underscore the sensitivity of motor or PT
to the details of the thresholding protocol, lighting condi-
Repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant differ-
tions, and eyes-open or eyes-closed state. This advocates
ences between thresholds [F(2,40) ¼ 2642, P < 0.001]. Post-
for brain mapping studies that use TMS to provide
hoc contrasts revealed significant differences between each
detailed statements of procedures, methodology, and TMS
pair of thresholds: (a) rMT vs. aMT: mean difference 6 SD
factors used when measuring reference thresholds and
¼ 9.33 6 3.98, P < 0.001, (b) rMT vs. PT: 13.76 6 9.59, P <
establishing experimental intensities over various brain
0.001, and (c) aMT vs. PT: 22.33 6 9.02, P < 0.0001.
There was a strong positive correlation between rMT
The use of different approaches toward threshold (up,
and aMT (r ¼ 0.92; P < 0.001). A significant positive corre-
down, or other) and different states of visual input (eyes
lation was also found between aMT and PT (r ¼ 0.53; P ¼
open or closed/blindfolded), employed in previous studies
0.014; Fig. 1a). A trend toward a correlation was present
during MT and PT determinations, may have limited the
between rMT and PT (r ¼ 0.43; P ¼ 0.052, Fig. 1b).
ability to find a correlation between the two measures. Our emphasis on systematic parallel methodology formeasuring MT and PT may have been critical in producing
DISCUSSION
All prior studies reported slightly different protocols for
In contrast to previous reports [Antal et al., 2003b; Bor-
approaching PT. Stewart et al. [2001b] determined PT by
oojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig et al., 2003; Stewart et al.,
decreasing or increasing TMS intensity in 5% increments
2001b], we found a significant correlation between aMT
from a starting point of 60% MSO. Two studies
and PT with a trend toward significance between rMT and
approached PT from below by increasing intensities in 1
PT. While the aMT and PT correlation was modest, with
or 2% increments [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al.,
aMT accounting for 27.8% of the group variance in PT, our
2002]. Gerwig et al. [2003] established PT by first in-
finding is the first to suggest such a relation between TMS
creasing intensities by 5% increments to then randomly
thresholds in visual and motor cortex. Such a correlation is
increasing and decreasing intensities by additional 2%
consistent with the idea that there is an element of global
excitability specific to each subject if the thresholding pro-
In contrast, details about MT procedures were relatively
cedures are similar for motor and visual cortex. While it
sparse. One study reported approaching MT downward
may be useful to determine TMS intensities with relevant
by reducing intensities by 2% increments [Stewart et al.,
region- and task-specific thresholds [Robertson et al.,
2001b]. Other studies provided few details about the
r Correlation Between Motor and Phosphene Thresholds r
approach to MT determination [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroo-
cortex showed greater activation and geniculate nucleus
jerdi et al., 2002; Stewart et al., 2001b]. Notably, while a
smaller activation under the eyes-open condition, visuomo-
downward approach to threshold has been recommended
tor structures (e.g., prefrontal and parietal cortices, frontal
for MT in recent guidelines [Rothwell et al., 1999], none of
eye fields, cerebellar vermis, thalamus, and basal ganglia)
the previous studies reported a downward approach to PT
revealed greater activation under the eyes-closed condi-
[Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig et al.,
tion. In addition, TMS studies have reported that, after
dark adaptation, motor cortex excitability is increased
Since both MEPs and phosphene perception show sub-
[Leon-Sarmiento et al., 2005], an effect comparable to the
stantial trial-to-trial variability, all MT and PT thresholding
decreased phosphene threshold after light deprivation
procedures in the present and prior studies have used a
[Boroojerdi et al., 2001], possibly due to GABAergic and
statistical endpoint of at least 50% detection of behavioral
glutamatergic mechanisms from corticocortical networks
outcome (MEP or phosphene perception) out of 6–10 test
connecting motor and visual areas [Bullier et al., 1996;
pulses. However, such statistical endpoints may produce
Fadiga et al., 2000; Leon-Sarmiento et al., 2005]. Thus, it
different thresholds depending on whether it is systemati-
seems apparent that the degree of light exposure and hav-
cally approached from below or from above. Conse-
ing eyes open or closed critically affects both brain activa-
quently, it has been proposed to define MT as the mean of
tion patterns and cortical excitabilty. However, unlike PT
two thresholds, one from above and one from below [Mills
[Boroojerdi et al., 2001], the time course of how MT might
and Nithi, 1997]. However, this method is time-consuming
vary with light deprivation is not known and it is unclear
and no more reliable than measuring thresholds by consis-
to what degree 15 min of dark-adaptation might have had,
tently approaching it downward [Tranulis et al., 2006].
if any, on our MT measurements. Because of the possibility
However, MTs obtained with different techniques in the
of different time-courses for dark-adaptation effects, we
same subject can differ by as much as 8% of MSO [Mills
chose to measure MT and PT consistently at 15 and
and Nithi, 1997]. Although comparable data regarding
45 minutes respectively after donning goggles rather than
differences in PT when approached differently are not
randomize the order of MT/PT determinations between
available, we explicitly adopted a consistently downward
threshold search for both MT and PT.
As expected and consistent with prior literature, we
Previous studies investigating MT and PT correlations
found significant differences between group mean rMTs,
performed PT determinations with either closed eyes
aMTs, and PTs. Within each subject, thresholds were con-
[Antal et al., 2003b] or blindfolds [Antal et al., 2003b; Bor-
sistently highest for PT and lowest for aMT. Because mus-
oojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig et al., 2003; Stewart et al.,
cle contraction reduces variability of the spinal excitability
2001b]. In contrast, lighting conditions during MT determi-
by ensuring suprathreshold activation of spinal motor neu-
nation procedures were either done with eyes open
rons, similar to other reports, we found that variability of
[Stewart et al., 2001b] or not reported [Antal et al., 2003b;
aMT was lower than rMT [Antal et al., 2003b; Nitsche
Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Gerwig et al., 2003; Stewart et al.,
et al., 2005]. Voluntary muscle contraction raises motor
2001b]. In either case, it is unlikely that explicit attention
cortical excitability (lower aMT), while reducing the spinal
was taken to ensure comparable visual exposure and eye-
contribution toward variability [Kiers et al., 1993]. In paral-
lid state during both MT and PT procedures.
lel, it is possible that raised visual cortical excitability
Although PT does not change significantly under brief
(lower PT) with dark-adaptation [Boroojerdi et al., 2000a]
exposure to different lighting conditions [Kammer and
may also reduce the variability of PT which is yet, to our
Beck, 2002], longer periods of dark adaptation reduce PT
knowledge, to be systematically examined. Thus, we note
and increase the yield of TMS induced phosphenes [Boroo-
that our correlation was identified between aMT and dark-
jerdi et al., 2000a; Marg and Rudiak, 1994]. We determined
adapted PT, both of which represent conditions of
PT after 45 min of darkness, a time point by which most
increased excitability and possibly of reduced variability
dark adaptation has taken place [Boroojerdi et al., 2000a].
or measurement noise. Kiers et al. [1993] suggested that
Even though dark adaptation may continue beyond this
during a relaxed muscle state, changes in cortical excitabil-
time point, PT determinations at the same point in time
ity in different regions may be relatively independent
allows reasonable comparisons between subjects. Since
whereas these changes may be positively correlated during
time of dark adaptation was not set in most other studies
voluntary muscle contraction. By changing tonic levels of
comparing MT and PT [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi
variability specific to each modality tested, i.e. for motor
et al., 2002; Gerwig et al., 2003; Stewart et al., 2001b], our
responses, removing spinal inhibition by muscle contrac-
data suggest that aMT may correlate mainly with a suffi-
tion; for visual percepts, increasing cortical excitability by
dark adaptation, noise in measurements can be reduced.
Even if no light is present, having eyes open or closed
Further, aMT and the PT we measured are likely related
differentially influences cortical network activation and
to the cortical elements or circuitry specific to the coil posi-
cortical excitability. An fMRI study demonstrated that acti-
tion and orientation used. Differences in magnetic stimula-
vation patterns differ between eyes-open and eyes-closed
tion parameters between PT and MT procedures also
conditions in darkness [Marx et al., 2004]. Whereas visual
potentially limit correlations within studies. In one study,
r Deblieck et al. r
different stimulator models which generate different wave-
excitability: people who see phosphenes were associated
form types were used for PT and MT determination [Antal
with greater BOLD activation in primary striate and
et al., 2003b]. Two studies used paired-pulse TMS over the
early extrastriate visual cortex, while those who do not
occipital lobe, while single-pulse TMS was used for MT
[Antal et al., 2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002]. For MT deter-
BOLD activation in higher extrastriate areas [Meister et al.,
minations, coil orientation was consistently oriented per-
pendicular to the central sulcus; for PT determinations, the
Although we matched many psychophysical aspects of
coil handle was held upward in three studies [Antal et al.,
MT and PT procedures, we recognize that our procedures
2003b; Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Stewart et al., 2001b] and lat-
were not purely parallel. MT was objectively quantified by
erally in one study [Gerwig et al., 2003]. To control for
MEP amplitudes, while PT depended on subjective report-
these factors, we employed the same stimulator and
ing. While the problem of phosphene perception is by
applied single-pulse TMS for all thresholding procedures.
nature a subjective one, future studies might more closely
Although recent studies suggest a lateral preference for
match MT procedures by asking subjects to report on their
current orientation for phosphene induction [Kammer,
perception of muscle twitches following TMS over the
1999], we used the cranio-caudal direction used in most
motor cortex. Scalp–brain distances also differ over motor
previous MT and PT studies [Antal et al., 2003b; Boroo-
and visual cortices and without MRI scans, we could not
jerdi et al., 2002; Stewart et al., 2001b].
adjust thresholds for scalp–brain distances as has been
Whether cortico-cortical or/and cortico-spinal axons are
proposed [Stokes et al., 2005]. We did not quantitatively
activated in the hand area depend on the orientation of
control force or baseline EMG amplitude for aMT, but
the TMS-applied magnetic field and the shape of the coil
rather monitored baseline EMG qualitatively online, while
[Di Lazzaro et al., 2003]. For example, with a biphasic
subjects squeezed with a pinch grip estimated at 10–15%
stimulator, both biphasic pulses may activate different de-
of maximal voluntary force, a level above which further
scending volleys depending on the stimulus intensity and
MEP facilitation is minimal [Mills, 1999]. While this proce-
the direction of current flow. When the current is charac-
dure is comparable to some routine assessments of aMT, it
terized by a lateromedial direction, such as was the case in
also likely introduces greater noise in the aMT measure-
this study, both the cortico-spinal and cortico-cortical
ment than if controlled. Future studies might control base-
axons are activated. Although it has been suggested that
line force or EMG more closely during aMT assessment. In
the level of muscle contraction does not affect the ampli-
addition, follow-up correlational studies that use two fully
tude of the D-wave induced by lateromedial magnetic
blinded investigators to measure thresholds may help fur-
stimulation, the level of excitability of the pyramidal tract
ther clarify this issue. With these limitations accounted for,
neurons lead to an increase in the size and number of the
it is possible our mild correlation between aMT and dark
I-wave volleys [Di Lazzaro et al., 2003]. Consequently, the
adapted PT could have been further strengthened.
finding that resting and active muscle states may recruit
Physiologic differences and similarities exist between
different axonal elements may also partially explain why
motor and visual cortices that may affect correlations
aMT, but not rMT was found correlated to PT. Similarly,
between PT and MT. For example, paired-pulse TMS stud-
PT is also sensitive to coil orientation with a preference
ies over the motor cortex show differential excitatory or
for an induced lateromedial current direction [Kammer
inhibitory effects dependent on the interstimulus interval
et al., 2001a], which suggests that the PT we determined
(ISI); in contrast, phosphene detection is facilitated with
is specific for our biphasic current and vertical coil orien-
paired-pulse TMS over visual cortices, independently of
ISI [Sparing et al., 2005]. Pharmacologic studies with TMS
Our correlation is limited to those volunteers who saw
have shown that MT can be affected by drugs that block
phosphenes. We found no significant differences in either
voltage-gated sodium and calcium channels (e.g., carbama-
aMT or rMT in the six subjects that did not see phos-
zepine, phenytoin, and lamotrigine) [Boroojerdi, 2002;
phenes versus those who did see phosphenes. While some
Chen et al., 1997b; Manganotti et al., 1999; Ziemann et al.,
studies report that all their subjects discerned phosphenes
1996b], while being unaffected by drugs that block GABA
[Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Rauschecker et al., 2004; Stewart
receptors (e.g., lorazepam, diazepam, vigabatrin) [Boroo-
et al., 2001b], other studies found a similar percentage of
jerdi, 2002; Inghilleri et al., 1996; Ziemann et al., 1996a,b].
subjects lacked phosphene perception [Antal et al.,
In contrast, these drug classes did not affect PT [Boroo-
2003a,b; Boroojerdi et al., 2000b; Meyer et al., 1991; Sparing
jerdi, 2002]. These differences continue to underscore the
et al., 2005]. It is not certain that subjects who do not see
use of region-specific thresholds when possible as refer-
phosphenes simply have higher than measurable thresh-
olds [Chronicle and Mulleners, 2004]. For example, it may
On the other hand, a common excitability across the
be differences in cortical anatomy or intrinsic differences
brain has been proposed in discussions of neurological dis-
in posterior cortical organization that could account some
orders with either increased or decreased excitability (e.g.,
subjects from not perceiving phosphenes. Meister et al.
exemplified by epilepsy as a disorder of hyperexcitability)
[2003] proposed that the difference in phosphene percep-
[Saugstad, 2005], and some neurophysiologic features of
tion may be found in different regions of visual cortical
motor and visual cortices have been found to be similar.
r Correlation Between Motor and Phosphene Thresholds r
For example, both MT and PT increase with hyperventila-
Artola A, Singer W (1987): Long-term potentiation and NMDA
tion [Sparing et al., 2007]; cortical excitability, as measured
receptors in rat visual cortex. Nature 330:649–652.
by either MT or PT, is reduced by 1 Hz rTMS over each
Bear MF (1996): Progress in understanding NMDA-receptor-de-
area [Boroojerdi et al., 2000b; Chen et al., 1997a]; and,
pendent synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex. J Physiol(Paris) 90:223–227.
rapid plasticity paradigms such as ischemic deafferentation
Boroojerdi B (2002): Pharmacologic influences on TMS effects.
over the motor cortex or dark-adaptation over the visual
cortices are both dependent on GABA and NMDA recep-
Boroojerdi B, Bushara KO, Corwell B, Immisch I, Battaglia F,
tors [Boroojerdi et al., 2001; Buetefisch et al., 2000; Zie-
Muellbacher W, Cohen LG (2000a): Enhanced excitability of the
mann et al., 2004]. Specifically, some features of synaptic
human visual cortex induced by short-term light deprivation.
plasticity [long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term
depression (LTD)], within both primary motor and visual
Boroojerdi B, Prager A, Muellbacher W, Cohen LG (2000b): Reduc-
cortex appear similar [Bear, 1996; Boroojerdi et al., 2001;
tion of human visual cortex excitability using 1 Hz transcranial
Buetefisch et al., 2000; Gilbert, 1998; Sanes and Donoghue,
magnetic stimulation. Neurology 54:1529–1531.
Boroojerdi B, Battaglia F, Muellbacher W, Cohen LG (2001): Mech-
2000; Ziemann et al., 2004]. In the motor cortex, LTP is
anisms underlying rapid experience-dependent plasticity in the
dependent on NMDA activation and reduced GABA-ergic
human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:14698–
inhibition [Hess and Donoghue, 1994; Otsuki et al., 1998],
while being unaffected by sodium and calcium channel
Boroojerdi B, Meister IG, Foltys H, Sparing R, Cohen LG, Toepper
modulation [Buetefisch et al., 2000; Otsuki et al., 1998].
R (2002): Visual and motor cortex excitability: A transcranial
Similarly, the direction of GABA-ergic inhibition and
magnetic stimulation study. Clin Neurophysiol 113:1501–1504.
NMDA receptor activation, essential for LTP, have been
Brasil-Neto JP, Cohen LG, Panizza M, Nilsson J, Roth BJ, Hallett
linked with visual plasticity as well [Artola and Singer,
M (1992): Optimal focal transcranial magnetic activation of the
1987; Bear, 1996; Quinlan et al., 1999], while being inde-
human motor cortex: Effects of coil orientation, shape of the
pendent of sodium channel blockade [Boroojerdi et al.,
induced current pulse, and stimulus intensity. J Clin Neuro-physiol 9:132–136.
2001]. This evidence of shared neurophysiologic features
Buetefisch CM, Davis BC, Wise SP, Sawaki L, Kopylev L, Classen
between motor and visual cortices may suggest a rationale
J, Cohen LG (2000): Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in
the human motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:3661–3665.
Bullier J, Schall JD, Morel A (1996): Functional areas in occipito-
CONCLUSION
frontal connections in the monkey. Behav Brain Res 76:89–97.
Chaudry V, Cornblath DR, Mellits ED, Avila O, Freimer ML, Glass
Using procedurally similar approaches toward thresh-
JD (1991): Inter- and intra-examiner reliability of nerve conduc-
olding, we report a significant correlation between aMT
tion measurements in normal subjects. Annal Neurol 30:841–
over motor cortex and dark-adapted PT over visual cortex.
This correlation provides evidence that a shared, signifi-
Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C, Celnik P, Wassermann EM, Hallett
cant global contribution to cortical responsiveness to TMS
M, Cohen LG. (1997a): Depression of motor cortex excitability
might be present over different cortical regions. Our data
by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology
suggest that TMS thresholds are sensitive to details of
thresholding procedure, lighting conditions, and eyes-
Chen R, Samii A, Canos M, Wassermann EM, Hallett M. (1997b):
Effects of phenytoin on cortical excitability in humans. Neurol-
open/eyes-closed state and invite a re-evaluation of meth-
ods of threshold determination when comparisons are
Chronicle EP, Mulleners WM (2004): Controversies in headache
being made across regions. Since aMT are easily measured,
[Letter to the Editor]. Cephalalgia 24:317–318.
our data suggest the possibility of using aMT as a guide to
Cohen LG, Celnik P, Pascual-Leone A, Corwell B, Faiz L, Dambro-
calibrate TMS intensities during TMS mapping studies
sia J, Honda M, Sadato N, Gerloff C, Catala MD, Hallett M
over the visual cortex and possibly other nonmotor cortical
(1997): Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind
regions. In addition, our correlation provides a rationale
for current guidelines that calibrate TMS intensity for re-
Desmurget M, Epstein CM, Turner RS, Prablanc C, Alexander GE,
petitive TMS applications based on MT regardless of corti-
Grafton ST (1999): Role of the posterior parietal cortex inupdating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat Neurosci
Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Pilato F, Mazzone P, Insola A, Ranieri
REFERENCES
F, Tonali PA (2003): Corticospinal volleys evoked by transcra-nial stimulation of the brain in conscious humans. Neurol Res
Antal A, Kincses TZ, Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2003a): Manipula-
tion of phosphene thresholds by transcranial direct current
Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2000): Visuomotor
stimulation in man. Exp Brain Res 150:375–378.
neurons: Ambiguity of the discharge or ‘motor’ perception? Int
Antal A, Nitsche MA, Kincses TZ, Lampe C, Paulus W (2003b):
No correlation between moving phosphene and motor thresh-
Gerwig M, Kastrup O, Meyer B-U, Niehaus L (2003): Evaluation
olds: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport
of cortical excitibilty by motor and phosphene thresholds in
transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurol Sci 215:75–78.
r Deblieck et al. r
Gilbert CD (1998): Adult cortical dynamics. Physiol Rev 78:467–
Modulating parameters of excitability during and after trans-
cranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex.
Gothe J, Brandt SA, Irlbacher K, Roericht S, Sabel BA, Meyer BU
(2002): Changes in visual cortex excitability in blind subjects as
Otsuki K, Morimoto K, Sato K, Yamada N, Kuroda S (1998):
demonstrated by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain
Effects of lamotrigine and conventional antiepileptic drugs on
amygdala- and hippocampal-kindled seizures in rats. Epilepsy
Heiser M, Iacoboni M, Maeda F, Marcus J, Mazziotta JC (2003):
The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. Eur J Neurosci
Quinlan EM, Philpot BD, Huganir RL, Bear MF (1999): Rapid, ex-
perience-dependent expression of synaptic NMDA receptors in
Hess G, Donoghue JP (1994): Long-term potentiation of horizontal
visual cortex in vivo. Natl Neurosci 2:352–357.
connections provides a mechanism to reorganize cortical motor
Rauschecker AM, Bestmann S, Walsh V, Thilo KV (2004): Phos-
maps. J Neurophysiol 71:2543–2547.
phene threshold as a function of contrast of external visual
Hotson JR, Anand S (1999): The selectivity and timing of motion
stimuli. Exp Brain Res 157:124–127.
processing in human temporo-parieto-occipital cortex: A trans-
Robertson EM, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2003): Studies in cog-
cranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 37:169–
nition: the problems solved and created by transcranial mag-
netic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 15:948–960.
Inghilleri M, Berardelli A, Marchetti P, Manfredi M (1996): Effects
Rothwell JC, Hallett M, Berardelli A, Eisen A, Rossini PM, Paulus
of diazepam, baclofen and thiopental on the silent period
W (1999): Magnetic stimulation: motor evoked potentials. The
evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation in humans. Exp
International Federation of Clinical Neuropysiology. Electroen-
cephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 52:97–103.
Kammer T (1999): Phosphenes and transient scotomas induced by
Sanes JN, Donoghue JP (2000): Plasticity and primary motor cor-
magnetic stimulation of the occipital lobe: Their topographic
tex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:393–415.
relationship. Neuropsychologia 37:191–198.
Saugstad LF (2005): The ‘‘new-old’’ way of thinking about brain
Kammer T, Beck S (2002): Phosphene thresholds evoked by trans-
disorder, cerebral excitability—The fundamental property of
cranial magnetic stimulation are insensitive to short-lasting
nervous tissue. Med Hypotheses 64:142–492.
variations in ambient light. Exp Brain Res 145:407–410.
Sparing R, Dambeck N, Stock K, Meister IG, Huetter D, Boroojerdi
Kammer T, Beck S, Erb M, Grodd W (2001a): The influence of
B (2005): Investigation of the primary visual cortex using short-
current direction on phosphene thresholds evoked by trans-
interval paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).
cranial magnetic stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 112:2015–
Sparing R, Dafotakis M, Buelte D, Meister IG, Noth J (2007): Excit-
Kiers L, Cros D, Chiappa KH, Fang J (1993): Variability of motor
ability of human motor and visual cortex before, during and
potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Electro-
after hyperventilation. J Appl Physiol 102:405–411.
encephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 89:415–423.
Stewart L, Ellison A, Walsh V, Cowey A. (2001a): The role of
Leon-Sarmiento FE, Bara-Jimenez W, Wassermann EM (2005): Vis-
transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in studies of vision,
ual deprivation effects on human motor cortex excitability.
attention and cognition. Acta Psychol 107:275–291.
Stewart LM, Walsh V, Rothwell JC. (2001b): Motor and phosphene
Manganotti P, Bongiovanni LG, Zanette G, Turazzini M, Fiaschi A
thresholds: A transcranial magnetic stimulation correlation
(1999): Cortical excitability in patients after loading doses of
study. Neuropsychologia 39:415–419.
lamotrigine: A study with magnetic brain stimulation. Epilep-
Stokes MG, Chambers CD, Gould IC, Henderson TR, Janko NE, Allen
NB, Mattingley JB (2005): Simple metric for scaling motor thresh-
Marg E, Rudiak D (1994): Phosphenes induced by magnetic stimu-
old based on scalp-cortex distance: Application to studies using
lation over the occipital brain: description and probable site of
transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurophysiol 94:4520–4527.
stimulation. Optom Vis Sci 71:301–311.
Tranulis C, Gueguen B, Pham-Scottez A, Vacheron MN, Cabel-
Marx E, Deutschlander A, Stephan T, Dieterich M, Wiesmann M,
guen G, Costantini A, Valero G, Calinovski A (2006): Motor
Brandt T (2004): Eyes open and eyes closed as rest conditions:
threshold in transcranial magnetic stimulation: Comparison of
Impact on brain activation patterns. Neuroimage 21:1818–1824.
three estimation methods. Neurophysiol Clin 36:1–7.
Meister IG, Weidemann J, Dambeck N, Foltys H, Sparing R,
Wassermann EM (1998): Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial
Krings T, Thron A, Boroojerdi B (2003): Neural correlates of
magnetic stimulation: Report and suggested guidelines from
phosphene perception. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol 56:305–311.
the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Trans-
Meyer BU, Diehl RR, Steinmetz H, Britton TC, Benecke R (1991):
cranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5–7, 1996. Electroencepha-
Magnetic stimuli applied over motor cortex and visual cortex:
Influence of coil position and field polarity on motor responses,
Ziemann U, Lonnecker S, Steinhoff BJ, Paulus W (1996a): The
phosphenes and eye movements. Electroencephalogr Clin Neu-
effect of lorazepam on the motor cortical excitability in man.
Mills KR (1999). Magnetic Stimulation of the Human Nervous Sys-
Ziemann U, Lonnecker S, Steinhoff BJ, Paulus W (1996b): Effects of
tem. New York: Oxford University Press.
antiepileptic drugs on motor cortex excitability in humans: A
Mills KR, Nithi K (1997): Corticomotor threshold to magnetic stim-
transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Ann Neurol 40:367–378.
ulation: Normal values and repeatability. Muscle Nerve 20:
Ziemann U, Iliac TV, Pauli C, Meintzschel F, Ruge D (2004):
Learning modifies subsequent induction of long-term potentia-
Nitsche MA, Seeber A, Frommann K, Klein CC, Rochford C, Nit-
tion-like and long-term depression-like plasticity in human
sche MS, Fricke K, Liebetanz D, Lang N, Antal A, et al. (2005):
motor cortex. J Neurosci 24:1666–1672.
COMITATO TECNICO-DIRETTIVO DMTE DELLA PROVINCIA DI CREMONA PROTOCOLLI OPERATIVI PER L’ ACCERTAMENTO DELL’ IDONEITA’ DEL DONATORE DI SANGUE E DI EMOCOMPONENTI E LA VALIDAZIONE DELLE UNITA’ RACCOLTE Premesse • Le Strutture Trasfusionali e le Associazioni di volontariato collaborano per mettere a disposizione di tutti i candidati donatori materiale educativo
containing amino acids such as methionine,cysteine and cystine. The resulting volatilereported complaint. Whether in the form ofoccasional morning breath, which nearlyevery otherwise healthy adult encounters,dimethyl sulphide and dimethyl disulphide)or rarer and more serious problems rangingfrom metabolic disorders to chest tumours,putrescine, foul-smelling diamines) are athalitosis