Copyright 2008 by theOxford Institute of AgeingAGEING HORIZONSIssue No. 8, 4–12
Age and Productivity Capacity: Descriptions, Causes and PolicyOptions
Vegard Skirbekk, International Institute for Applied Systems Analysis Abstract
understand which policies individuals and governmentscould undertake to increase the retirement age.
This article reviews how work performance differs over thelife cycle by describing and discussing findings from variousapproaches. This includes managers’ evaluations, the quan-
The Shape of the Age-Productivity Curve
tity and quality of goods and services produced by workersof different ages, the performance of age-mixed teams, to
Identifying exceptional achievement at early or late ages is
what extent the age distribution of employees depends on
not difficult. For example, Goethe published the second part
the type of work and how the age distribution changes due
of Faust only in 1832 when he was 82 years old, while James
to technological change and business cycle shocks, analy-
Watson discovered the DNA molecule in 1953 at the age of
ses of employer-employee datasets, descriptions of
25 with a few remarkable findings afterwards. Classic texts
age-earnings profiles in settings where they could reflect
in the age-performance literature, such as Crichton-Browne
performance and the output of researchers and artists over
(1905) and Osler (1906), are biased as they focus on excep-
the life cycle. The causes of productivity variation by age
tional performance by either senior or younger individuals to
are also considered, with a particular focus on experience
support their hypotheses of either an early or late productiv-
and cognitive abilities. The findings suggest that produc-
ity peak. Moreover, they only look at age performance among
tivity tend to increase during the initial years in the labourmarket before it stabilizes and often declines towards the
a small creative elite and use this to infer age variation in
end of the working life. Productivity reductions at older
performance for the population as a whole.
ages are strongest in job tasks where problem solving, learn-ing and speed are important, while for work tasks where
Classic studies of elite achievement by researchers, inno-
experience and verbal abilities matter more, there is less or
vators and artists by age tend to suggest that productivity
no reduction in productivity among elderly workers. Trends
peaks in the 30s and 40s, with substantially lower output
in the age-productivity relation are discussed in relation to
at younger and older ages, also when quality (e.g., journal
changing work tasks and job requirements, combined with
article citations) is controlled for (Lehman, 1953; Cole,
changes in the requirement of skills (decline in demand for
1979; Simonton, 1988; Miller, 1999; Kanazawa, 2003;
physical strength, increase in the need to learn new skills).
Oster and Hamermesh, 1998). Further evidence suggesting
Policies that could be considered to raise productivity among
that there is a negative association between scientific output
senior workers include on-the-job training, education andpromotion of health. However, a later retirement could also
and age from relatively early in adult life is given in studies
raise incentives to update one’s own skills and work harder
of economists and scientists by Bayer and Dutton (1977)
at older ages (which may be achieved through pension
and Bratsberg et al. (2003). However, a relatively flat
reforms and wage liberalisation). Moreover, a better age-
output profile for individuals aged 30–59 for 19th century
mix in the workplace, allowing older and younger
scientists is found by Dennis (1956), and for a limited
individuals to benefit from their comparative advantages,
sample of 20th century researchers by McDowell (1982).
is likely to improve overall productivity in ageing nations.
Nobel Prize Laureates do their most important contributions
Introduction
in their 30s, according to a study by Jones (2005). The onset
Extending the working life is a popular response to popu-
of the most innovate age phase, however, increased by about
lation ageing in order to maintain economic prosperity and
6 years over the 20th century (which he suggests could be due
sustainability. To achieve this, various policies are being
to a longer training period as the knowledge base has
considered, including changes in age-specific pension enti-
expanded). The decline in innovative output, however, is
tlements and age-discrimination laws (in particular,
found to be age specific and constant which implies that the
repealing laws for dismissing older workers without justi-
plateau of peak performance has narrowed over time.
fication). However, attempts to extend the age at retirementwill only be effective to the extent that productive employ-
Entrepreneurial activity, start-up of new firms or expansions
ment can be extended. This is important in order to ensure
of existing ones, are more likely to be carried out by rela-
that increased retirement age does not simply translate into
tively young adults, according to findings from the 34
periods of unemployment. In this context, taking age-
countries surveyed in the Global Entrepreneurship Monitor
specific productivity into account is necessary in order to
(GEMConsortium, 2004). Peak entrepreneurial activity is
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
found among individuals aged 25–44. In support of a young
Czaja and Sharit (1993) investigate the extent to which age
entrepreneurial peak, Aubert et al. (2007) find, in a French
has an impact on computer-based work performance and
study, that the more innovative a firm is, the higher the
find that increased age was associated with longer response
wages of younger employees are relative to wages of other
times and a greater number of errors for all tasks consid-
ered. These task-quality/speed tests are potentially moreobjective as they do not rely on subjective assessment.
In order to describe age variation in performance of the
However, they may be biased from the fact that the workers
average worker rather than elite groups, one needs a differ-
are selected in terms of age groups and occupational types
ent set of productivity measures. Supervisors’ ratings are
(Rubin and Perloff, 1993). Further, the time-limit common
often used to identify the relation between the employee’s
in such studies may bias results. For example older employ-
age and his or her productivity. McEvoy and Cascio (1989)
ees may maintain a higher work speed in the short period
review 96 studies on the impact of the employee’s age on
they are studied than what they would be able to do in a
supervisors’ assessment and sales records and find no clear
normal job situation (Salthouse and Maurer, 1996).
effect of age on productivity. Likewise, reviews by Warr(1994) and Waldman and Avolio (1986), based primarily
A recent estimation approach is based on analyses of
on supervisor assessment, find no or a slightly negative
employer-employee datasets (e.g., Gelderblom and de
impact of age on job performance. However, Remery et al.
Koning, 2002; Hægeland and Klette, 1999). A key strength
(2003) find that older individuals are seen as less produc-
of these estimates are the large samples, which can encom-
tive in particular in firms with a higher proportion of senior
pass most workers in some of the main industries in a
workers, which is where knowledge about older individu-
country; several samples include a few million individuals
als’ work capacities is likely to be highest, in a survey 1007
and thousands of firms. The most common finding from
these studies is a hump-shaped relation between job perform-ance and age. An overview over analyses of the effect of
A general problem with most approaches used to measure
age on productivity using employer-employee datasets are
age variation among workers is the sample selection
given in the Annex. Of the 14 studies considered, 11 find
problem. Good workers get promoted, while inefficient
a productivity decline in the 50s relative to the 30s and 40s,
workers may lose their jobs. Hence, positive selection can
two have inconsistent results, while one finds that produc-
increase by age, which could lead to bias in the estimates
tivity peaks among the oldest workers. However, bias may
of the oldest age groups’ working capacity. Self-assessed
come from the fact that many determinants of a firm’s
“work ability” is sometimes considered, and estimates from
value-added, such as capital levels, are either omitted or
Finland suggest a clear decline by age of workers (van
poorly measured. The reverse causality problem could also
Ours et al. 2007). Solem (2006) presents evidence from
be an issue – a company’s success can increase the number
Norwegian surveys on subjective general work ability that
of new employees and lead to a younger age structure,
shows a decline from the 30s to the 60s. Nevertheless,
which could mean that a young age structure could be the
respondents believe that they are equally capable of perform-
consequence rather than the cause of a company’s success.
ing their work over the same age interval; although generalwork ability declines with age. This may be because rele-
Wages are often determined by other factors than individual
vant abilities are fully maintained and the worker is
contribution to the firms’ value-added, including the role of
increasingly well matched to the type of work task he/she
unions, uncertainty about new workers productivity and
performs. A particular problem with managers’ ratings and
delayed payment contracts where performance while being
self-evaluations is that these evaluations can be biased as
young results in higher earnings for those still employed while
they are subjective. Evaluations of older workers could
they are older (Agell and Lundborg, 1995; Freeman, 1982;
therefore be inflated due to loyalty concerns and as a reward
Hutchens, 1989). Age-earnings profiles can, however,
for past achievement – or in the case of self-reports, as a
provide information on productivity differences in settings
means of self-justification, or even self-degradation.
where wages are likely to reflect actual productivity. One
Discriminatory attitudes among managers towards older or
example is a study by Lazear and Moore (1984) who examine
younger individuals can also affect an age-productivity esti-
the difference between earnings profiles of the self-employed
mate, which reduces the validity of this approach (Levy,
and salary workers. Kotlikoff and Gokhale (1992) study age-
variation in the wages offered when firms hire newemployees. Their findings suggest managers have earliest
Measuring the impact of age on job performance is some-
productivity maximum, while office and sales-workers have
times based on measures of the quantity and quality of a
somewhat older peaks, though they all reach their highest
worker’s output. Studies based on this approach tend to find
productivity in the 40s, with sharp declines thereafter.
that older employees have lower productivity levels. Astudy of a several industries from the U.S. Department of
A study by Boot (1995) describes age-earnings profiles for
Labor (1957) finds that job performance increases until the
British workers in the first half of the 19th century, when
age of 35 and steadily declines thereafter. At the end of the
there were few regulations in the labour market. For the
career, productivity declines by 14% in the men’s footwear
physically demanding work analyzed here, men reached
industry, and 17% in the household furniture industry.
their peak earnings in their early 30s, and wages decreased
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
substantially from around 40 years of age. In a similar
suggest that older workers have a modest negative impact on
study, Johnson (2003) looks at British manual workers earn-
productivity growth, while Nishimura et al. (2002) find that
ings from the 1830s to the 1930s, and finds a stable
the share of well-educated workers above 40 years raised tech-
age-earnings pattern where wages reach their peak in the
nological progress in the 1980s, but decreased it in the 1990s
mid-30s and remain stable or decline slightly thereafter.
when investigating technical progress and growth in Japan1980–1998. It is not clear why certain relations between the
Labour force attachment trends can also reveal information
age structure and economic growth levels exist. They could be
about age differences in productivity. If older workers cope
due to other factors than age variation in work performance,
less well with changes in the workplace, then rapid changes
such as high savings levels among certain age groups (i.e.,
should affect them worse than younger age groups. Bartel
higher capital levels could raise growth) or omitted factors in
and Sicherman (1993) put forward evidence that the risk of
the regressions (which both affect economic growth as well as
job loss is in fact greater among older workers when the
mortality, fertility or migration levels).
rate of technological change is highest. This finding is alsosupported in studies based on inter-sectoral data (Ahituv and
Causes of Age Variation in Work
Zeira, 2000). Technological improvements may also dispro-
Performance Relation
portionally benefit the young, and in a study of Germanfirms, Hujer and Radic (2005) find that older highly-skilled
Workers’ productivity levels differ by age for many reasons,
workers benefit less from technological improvements
including the length of work experience, cognitive func-
tioning, education, physical abilities, stamina, health, familyand care obligations, motivation, energy, matching of the
Age diversity has been suggested to have a positive effect on
worker to the task, loyalty and personality.
productivity through better “age-matching” between sales-persons and customers and through complementarity of
Average muscle strength decreases by roughly 10% per
different age groups, but the evidence seems inconclusive.
decade for ages 20–60, by approximately 15% each decade
Experimental research suggests cooperation is highest when
for ages 60–80, and 30% each decade after age 80 (Mazzeo,
juniors and seniors are mixed together (Charness and Villeval,
2000). De Zwart et al. (1995) show that aerobic capacity
2006). However, Leonard and Levine (2003) considered 800
peaks somewhere in the 20s and declines by around 1% per
retail stores employing 70,000 individuals and found that age
year. Flexibility decreases with age, which makes it diffi-
diversity was negatively related to sales. Hamilton et al.
cult to adopt certain working positions (Bosek et al., 2005).
(2004) studied the productivity of workers in a garment plantthat had shifted from individual piece rate payments to team
Moreover, cognitive skills have been shown to be increas-
piece rate payments, and again found that greater diversity
ingly important to work tasks over time, as both the work
in age was associated with lower productivity gains. A
intensity and the industrial composition change over time,
European study on age diversity suggests that while in a
while physical strength is becoming decreasingly important
German car assembly plant, age diversity increases the prob-
(Broadberry, 1997; Spitz-Oener, 2006; Skirbekk, 2008).
ability of calling in sick, evidence from Finnish firms show
Cognitive ability test scores have been found to be more
a positive effect of age diversity (van Ours et al. 2007). Van
closely correlated with labour market performance than any
Ours et al. suggest that this seeming contradiction is due to
other observable characteristics among job candidates
different levels of observation, where age diversity at the
team level (German case) may be negative (as age diversitymay imply impeded communication), while diversity at the
Some cognitive abilities decline with age, while others tend
plant level (Finnish case) may mean that in some parts of the
to be relatively robust over the life cycle. A division can
plant (e.g., management) there are many older (wise, expe-
be drawn between crystallised abilities, which remain at a
rienced) workers while in other parts (e.g., production), there
high functional level until late in life, and fluid abilities,
are many younger (fit, flexible) workers. Hence, comple-
mental abilities that are strongly reduced over the life span
mentarity at the firm level may well be consistent with the
(Horn and Cattell, 1966; Verhaegen and Salthouse, 1997).
Crystallised abilities depend on accumulated knowledge,and includes semantic meaning and vocabulary size. The
Several macroeconomic investigations find a relation between
second group, fluid abilities such as perceptual speed and
national/regional age structure and economic growth.
reasoning abilities, tend to relate to performance and speed
However, results diverge and while Brunow and Hirte (2006)
of solving tasks related to new materials.
find that 30–44 year olds boost per capita output growth acrossEuropean regions, Feyrer (2005) finds that the 40–49 year old
Schwartzman et al. (1987) find that verbal skills (crys-
age group is the strongest associated with productivity growth
tallised abilities) remain virtually unchanged at older ages,
using a large panel of developed and developing countries,
while reasoning and speed (fluid abilities) decrease from
while Lindh and Malmberg (1999) and Malmberg and Lindh
early adulthood, based on psychometric test results of men
(2002) find that the share of 50–64 year olds is associated with
in different age groups. In a test-retest study of twins, Blum
economic growth in the OECD from 1850–1990. Moreover,
et al. (1970) provide similar findings: vocabulary size is
Tang and MacLeod (2006), using Canadian regional data,
observed to remain constant from young to old ages despite
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
a general reduction in other cognitive abilities. A decline
nity of part-time work increases the opportunities for active
over the working life span in some mental abilities has
labour force participation among the elderly. These types of
been found to be similar for both men and women, and the
flexible work settings contrast the situation for assembly line
same patterns are found across different countries and
workers in the past, where constant and continuous attention
cultures (Maitland et al., 2000; Park et al., 1999).
Furthermore, individuals with high ability levels are subjectto the similar changes in cognitive functioning as those
Improving Productivity and Employment at
with low ability levels (Baltes and Mayer, 1999; Deary etOlder Ages al., 2000). Age-related reductions in memory and learning
Romans sometimes described old age as mala aetas, the bad
capabilities have been documented among many non-human
age, and young age as bona aetas, the good age (Parkin,
species, ranging from fruit flies to primates (Bunk, 2000;
2003). However, age-related changes to physique and mind
have changed since Roman times, and cognitive and healthdeterioration takes place at increasingly later ages in contem-
In some occupations, the cognitive abilities that remain
porary societies. And surroundings have changed, where the
stable are the ones most closely correlated to job success.
introduction of technologies compensating declines in
Senior employees can remain highly productive within a
sensory and muscular capabilities, electric wheelchairs,
field that they know well and where relatively long expe-
more user-friendly computer systems, effective medicines
rience is beneficial. Tacit knowledge, procedural knowledge
against pain and hypertension and working at home oppor-
used to solve everyday problems, tends not to decrease at
tunities allow one to continue to function professionally in
older ages. The age-robustness of this ability could explain
older ages in a large number of work places.
why many older managers perform as good as youngerones (Colonia-Willner, 1998). However, when performing
There is evidence that mental abilities have improved over
unfamiliar work, workers have to rely on the ability to
the course of the last century (Tuddenham, 1948; Flynn,
learn and to adjust exactly those skills that decline most with
1987; Neisser, 1997). Improvement in cognitive skills have
age. Senior individuals are less able than young individu-
been found to also take place for individuals aged 50–79
als to reorient themselves to new task requirements and to
during the last decades (Romeu Gordo, 2005) which is
solve novel problems (Smith, 1996) and age-induced produc-
likely to be related to rapid growth in education and on-
tivity reductions may increase with the complexity of new
the-job training (Lutz et al., 2007; Riphahn and
work tasks (Myerson et al., 1990). Hence, staying within
Trübswetter, 2006). Individuals from younger cohorts are
one field of expertise relates to high productivity, while
likely to be more trained and motivated when taking ability
changing between fields can lower productivity much more
tests, as such tests are increasingly being used in job candi-
(e.g., Cole, 1979; Rybash et al., 1986).
date selection processes (Jenkins, 2001).
Experience is often quoted by employers as one of the most
Paralleling the sustained decline in mortality (which has
important determinants of job performance (Bellmann, 2007;
taken place for at least two centuries in Northern Europe),
Golini, 2004). Salthouse (1984) uses typists as an example
disability-free life expectancies have increased (Crimmins
of a profession where experience alleviates the impact of
et al., 1997; Manton et al., 1997; Lee, 2003; Romeu
cognitive reductions. He finds that older typists compen-
Gordo, 2005; Schoeni et al., 2001). The morbidity decrease
sate for their reduced speed through the use of more efficient
is partly related to less physically demanding work tasks.
work strategies compared to their younger counterparts.
Costa et al. (1986) argues that 29% of the observed decline
Ericsson and Lehmann (1996) argue that it takes roughly
in chronic disease rates and 75% of the decline in back prob-
10 years to achieve expert competence in games and situ-
lems in the 20th century was caused by a shift to non-manual
ations where strategic and analytic competence is important,
labour and a reduction in the physical load. Not only has
such as chess. The 10-year estimate is also supported by
work become less strenuous, but estimates put forward by
findings from a variety of job domains, ranging from live-
Ausubel and Grubler (1995) suggest that in France,
stock evaluation and X-ray analysis to scientific performance
Germany, the UK, USA and Japan, the number of hours
in medical and natural sciences (Phelps and Shanteau, 1978;
worked per year declined by at least a third from 1850 to
Lehman, 1953; Lesgold, 1984; Raskin, 1936).
1987, which is likely to imply a substantial reduction in thehealth requirements to work.
A Norwegian survey (Econ, 1998) shows that only 0.6% ofthe employers preferred workers with a length of experience
To extend the productive working life at older ages, a
above 10 years. As the average age of entering the labour
government could follow many different policies. Taxes,
force in the OECD is in the early 20s (depending on educa-
earnings and pension systems need to be restructured in
tional levels, OECD, 1999), this would suggest that 99.4%
order to increase incentives for older individuals to continue
of the employers find workers sufficiently experienced in the
to work rather than to retire. However, the question is not
30s, which would exclude experience as a major advantage
only about creating financial incentives for a long and
between workers in the 50s and 60s compared to workers in
productive working life, but also about the need to alter
the 30s and 40s, although all have an advantage compared to
norms, beliefs and behaviour that affect labour force partic-
workers in their 20s. The increased prevalence and opportu-
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
Many elderly would like to continue working when they reach
proportion of the age group remains active. Socialising has
retirement age, while employers are often hesitant to employ
been shown to be good for mental health and for many,
them, making it difficult to increase retirement ages. High
particularly men, the work place is the key place to meet
costs are often seen as the main disadvantage of older workers
others (Cohen, 2004; Melchior et al., 2003).
(Golini, 2004; Munnell et al., 2006). Pension payments andhealth insurance often increase by age, and as wages may not
As the current population is likely to be urban, have seden-
be adjusted for this, it implies that older individuals are cost-
tary occupations, and use motorized transport, exercise
lier for firms (Scott et al. 1995). In firms with steep wage
should be encouraged through increased availability of green
profiles and pension benefits, employment opportunities have
space and sports facilities. More exercise can increase work
been found to be worse for elderly in the US (Hirsch et al.,
capacity at older ages. James and Coyle (1998) find that
2000), Hong Kong (Heywood et al., 1999) and the UK
regular exercise improves the working memory function
among older men, while obesity has been found to havenegative effects on cognitive performance net of education,
For example, in Japan, it is customary for those who retire
occupation, cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption, total
from a normal working career to enter a lower-wage but
cholesterol, and a diagnosis of diabetes (Elias et al., 2003;
relatively prestigious second career in their 60s (Clark et
Geroldi et al., 2003; Gustafson et al., 2003).
al., 2006). Ichino et al. (2006) find that when displacedolder workers’ minimum wages they are willing to accept
Encouraging new nutritional habits and increased exercise
fall faster than those of prime-age workers, they catch up
could lower the negative impacts on productivity, such as
in terms of employment. A comparison of earnings levels
absenteeism, disability, workplace injuries and health costs
and employment rates across the OECD suggest that wage
(Schmier et al., 2006; Colditz, 1999). Cereals and fish, red
flexibility and willingness to accept lower wages relates to
wine or even aspirin could possibly strengthen cognitive
functioning (Nilsson et al., 2003; Solfrizzi et al., 2003). Governments could also impose stronger non-smoking legis-
However employers/governments also need to make funda-
lation, as smoking has been found to negatively affect
mental changes to how they view older workers. Flexible
cognitive functioning, also when adjusting for the negative
retirement and part-time positions, improved career planning
selection of smokers (Deary et al., 2003). Moreover, better
and more internal mobility, preventive measures and occu-
system design can reduce computer problems for older
pational health programmes could increase work productivity
of the elderly (see Lindley et al., 2006 for “good practice”examples). Age discrimination must end, job training oppor-
In spite of the seemingly unavoidable age-related reductions
tunities should be given without age constraints, and
in cognitive abilities, targeted training programmes and
age-specific dismissal legislation should be phased out. One
engagement in complex work tasks may provide a way of
could, if possible, reorganize to allow the elderly to perform
halting the decline. Schooler et al. (1999) find that the degree
work tasks where they have their comparative advantages,
of job complexity is associated with the level of mental func-
which is where experience, management and communication
tioning at older ages, which could be due to continued learning
skills are important rather than tasks which require high
in the workplace. Schaie and Willis (1986) conclude that train-
processing speed and rapid reorientation.
ing programmes can stabilise, or even reverse, age-relateddeclines in inductive reasoning and spatial orientation among
More pensioners means that taxes need to be increased to
many individuals. Similar evidence is presented by Ball et al.
support more pensioners, and higher taxes slow the creation
(2002), who find that persons who exercise the use of indi-
of jobs. However, a change of norms is necessary, where
vidual abilities such as speed, reasoning and memory, enhance
a higher pension age should become the custom, and seen
the functional level of these abilities. Katzman (1993) argues
as necessary and fair. Further, beliefs that justify early
that participating in educational courses increases synaptic
retirement, e.g., that retiring early increases job opportu-
density in the neocortical association cortex, and could there-
nities for the young, should be abandoned. In countries
fore delay the onset of dementia by up to 4–5 years. Using a
such as the Netherlands, France, Germany, Spain, Italy
British industry dataset from 1983–1996, Dearden et al.
and the UK, between 40% and 70% of the individuals agree
(2006) find that training is associated with productivity
that “older people should be forced to retire when jobs are
increases, and more so than what wage increases would
scarce” (van Ours et al., 2007).
suggest. Likewise, in separate studies of German firm panel-datasets, Zwick (2002) and Kuckulenz and Maier (2006)
If one expects a later retirement, one’s training investment
identify large productivity increases in response to the train-
decisions are likely to change. A longer remaining working
life implies that it will be optimal for individuals to investmore in updating skills and to increase work efforts at older
Conclusion
ages. When the elderly expect to work longer, they will haveincentives to invest more in training and to work harder.
The evidence put forward in this article suggests that job
They will also continue to update, maintain and extend
performance often does decrease at older ages, but not
their professional networks, which will be easier if a larger
for all work tasks and less so in occupations where abil-
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
ities that do not decline by age are important. Moreover,
the elderly to benefit from their comparative advantages
the elderly’s productive potential is likely to have
and effectively extend the working life. Changes in
increase over time as modern jobs decreasingly rely on
incentive systems, regulations and habits that lead to less
strength, cognitive abilities and health of the elderly
smoking, better nutrition and more exercise could further
improve, and part-time flexible work arrangements have
decrease disability levels and raise cognitive functioning
become commonplace. More training, better work organ-
is likely to help increase work performance levels and
isation, and more flexible earnings systems could allow
labour force participation at older ages.
References
Agell, J. and P. Lundborg (1995) Theories of pay and unemployment:
Transfers, and the Macroeconomy, Tokyo, Japan.
Survey evidence from Swedish manufacturing firms. Scandinavian
Cohen, S. (2004) Social relationships and health. AmericanJournal of Economics, 97, pp. 295–307. Psychologist, November, pp. 676–684.
Ahituv, A. and J. Zeira (2000) Technical progress and early
Colditz, G. A. (1999) Economic costs of obesity and inactivity. Med.
retirement. CEPR Discussion Paper No. 2614. London: Centre for
Sci. Sports Exerc., 31(11), pp. 663–S667.
Cole, S. (1979) Age and scientific performance. American Journal of
Aubert, P. and B. Crépon (2004) La productivité des salariés âgés: une
Sociology, 84(4), pp. 958–977.
tentative d’estimation [Productivity of older workers: a tentative
Colonia-Willner, R. (1998) Practical intelligence at work: Relationship
estimate]. Économie et statistique, 368, pp. 95–119.
between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank
Aubert, P., E. Caroli, and M. Roger (2007) New technologies,
environment. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), pp. 45–57.
organisation and age: Firm level evidence. The Economic Journal,
Costa, P. C., R. R. McRae, A. B. Zonderman, H. E. Barbano, B.
Lebowitz, and D. M. Larson. (1986) Cross-sectional studies of
Ausubel, J. H. and A. Gruebler (1995) Working less and living longer:
personality in a national sample: Stability in neuroticism,
Long-term trends in working time and time budgets. Technological
extraversion and openness. Psychology and Aging, 1, pp. 144–149. Forecasting and Social Change, 50 (3), pp. 113–131.
Crépon, B., N. Deniau, and S. Perez-Duarte (2002) Wages,
Ball, K., D. B. Berch, K. F. Helmers, J. B. Jobe, M. D. Leveck, M.
productivity and worker characteristics. A French perspective.
Marsiske, J. N. Morris, G.W. Rebok, D. M. Smith, S. L.
Mimeo. Paris: National Institute for Statistics and Economic Studies
Tennstedt, F.W. Unverzagt, and S. L.Willis (2002) Effects of
cognitive training interventions with older adults. Journal of the
Crichton-Browne, J., (1905) The Prevention of Senility and a SanitaryAmerican Medical Association, 288(18), pp. 2271–2281.
Baltes, P. and K. U. Mayer (eds.) (1999) The Berlin aging study:
Crimmins, E. M., Y. Saito, and S. L. Reynolds (1997) Further
Aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
evidence on recent trends in the prevalence and incidence of
Barrington, L. and K. Troske (2001) Workforce diversity and
disability among older Americans from two sources: The LSOA and
productivity: An analysis of employer-employee matched data. New
the NHIS. Journal of Gerontology, 52B(2), pp. 59–71.
York: Conference Board Economics Program Working Paper Series
Czaja, S. and C. C. Lee (2007) The impact of aging on access to
technology. Universal Access in the Information Society, 5(4), pp.
Bartel, A. P. and N. Sicherman (1993) Technological change and
retirement decisions of old workers. Journal of Labor Economics,
Czaja, S. and J. Sharit (1993) Age differences in the performance of
computer-based work. Psychol. Aging, 8(1), pp. 59–67.
Bayer, A. E. and J. E. Dutton (1977) Career, age and research
Daniel, K. and J. Heywood (2007) The determinants of hiring older
professional activities of academic scientists. Journal of Higher
workers: UK evidence. Labour Economics, 14, pp. 35–51. Education, 48(3), pp. 252–282.
de Zwart, B., M. Frings-Dresen, and F. van Dijk. (1995) Physical
Bellmann, L. (2007) Einstellungen Älterer aus betrieblicher Sicht.
workload and the ageing worker: A review of the literature.
[Firms’ attitudes towards elderly]. Paper presented at the Conference
International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health,
“Chancen für Ältere? Neue Forschungsergebnisse zur
Arbeitsmarktsituation Älterer” held on March 2, 2007 in Berlin.
Dearden, L., H. Reed, and J. Van Reenen (2006) The impact of
Blum, J. E., L. F. Jarvik, and E. T. Clark (1970) Rate of change on
training on productivity and wages: Evidence from British Panel
selective tests of intelligence: A twenty-year longitudinal study of
Data. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 68(4), pp.
aging. Journal of Gerontology, 25, pp. 171–176.
Boot, H. M. (1995) How skilled were Lancashire cotton factory
Deary, I. J., A. Pattie, M. D. Taylor, M. C. Whiteman, J. M. Starr,
workers in 1833? Economic History Review, XLVIII (2), pp.
and L. J. Whalley (2003) Smoking and cognitive change from age
11 to age 80. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 74, pp. 1006–1007.
Bosek, M., B. Grzegorzewski, A. Kowalczyk, and I. Lubinski (2005)
Deary, I. J., L. J. Whalley, H. Lemmon, J. R. Crawford, and J. M.
Degradation of postural control system as a consequence of Parkinson’s
Starr (2000) The stability of individual differences in mental ability
disease and ageing. Neuroscience Letters, 376, pp. 215–20.
from childhood to old age. Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish mental
Bratsberg, B., J .F. Ragan, Jr., and J. T. Warren (2003) Negative
survey. Intelligence, 28(1), pp. 49–55.
returns to seniority: New evidence in academic markets. Industrial
Dennis, W. (1956) Age and productivity among scientists. Science, 27,
and Labor Relations Review, 56(2), pp. 306–323.
Broadberry, S. N. (1997) Forging up, falling behind and catching up.
ECON (1998) Arbeidsmarkedspolitikken overfor eldre arbeidssøkere
A sectoral analysis of Anglo-American productivity differences,
[Labour Market Policies towards elderly job seekers]. Oslo,
1870–1990. Research in Economic History, 17, pp. 1–37.
Norway: Report for the Ministry of Local Government and Regional
Brunow, S. and G. Hirte (2006) Age structure and regional growth.
Development. Econ Report No. 98/97.
Jahrbuch für Regionalwissenschaft/Review of Regional Research,
Elias, M. F., P. K. Elias, L. M. Sullivan, P. A. Wolf, and R. B.
d’Agostino (2003) Lower cognitive function in the presence of
Bunk, S. (2000) Cognition and aging. A rare colony of old macaques
obesity and hypertension: the Framingham heart study. International
gives clues to age impairment. The Scientist, 14, pp. 18–19. Journal of Obesity 27, pp.260–268.
Charness, G. and M.-C.Villeval (2006) Cooperation, competition, and
Ericsson, K. A. and A. C. Lehmann (1996) Expert and exceptional
risk attitudes: An intergenerational field and laboratory experiment.
performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints.
Institute for the Study of Labour (IZA) Discussion Paper 2574. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, pp. 273–305.
Clark, R., N. Ogawa, and R. Matsukura (2006) Population aging,
Feyrer, J. (2005) Demographics and productivity. Draft. Hanover,
Changing retirement policies, and lifetime earnings profiles. Paper
New Hampshire: Dartmouth College, Department of Economics.
presented at the Conference on Population Aging, Intergenerational
Flynn, J. R. (1987) Massive IQ gains in 14 nations. What IQ tests
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, pp. 171–191. Paper 11359. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic
Freeman, R. B. (1982) Union wage practices and wage dispersion
within establishments. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 36,
Kanazawa, S. (2003) Why productivity fades with age: The crime-genius
connection. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, pp. 257–272.
Gelderblom, A. and J. de Koning (2002) Exclusion of older workers,
Katzman, R. (1993) Education and the prevalence of dementia and
productivity and training. In: K. Schömann and P.J. O’Connell
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43, pp. 13–20.
(eds.) Education, Training and Employment Dynamics. Cheltenham:
Kotlikoff, K. and J. Gokhale (1992) Estimating a firm’s age-
productivity profile using the present value of workers’ earnings. The
GEMConsortium (2004) National team reports. Accessed 21 April 2005
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(4), pp. 1215–1242.
at http://www.gemconsortium.org/category_list.asp?cid=164
Kuckulenz, A. and M. Maier (2006) Heterogeneous returns to training.
Geroldi, C., L. Ferrucci, S. Bandinelli, et al. (2003) Mild cognitive
An analysis with German data using local instrumental variables.
deterioration with subcortical features. Prevalence, clinical
ZEW Discussion Paper No. 06–002. Mannheim, Germany: Centre
characteristics, and association with cardiovascular risk factors in
community-dwelling older persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc., 51, pp.
Lazear, E. P. and R. L. Moore (1984) Incentives, productivity, and labor
contracts. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 99(2), pp. 275–296.
Golini, A. (2004) Demographic trends, aging and labour: The work of the
Lee, R. (2003) The demographic transition: Three centuries of
young elderly: Is there willingness and possibility? Paper presented at
fundamental change. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17, pp.
the Vienna Demographic Colloquium on February 23, 2004.
Grund, C. and N. Westergård-Nielsen (2005) Age structure of the
Lehman, H. C. (1953) Age and Achievement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
workforce and firm performance.” Working paper WP-05–10.
Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus School of Business.
Leonard, J. S. and D. I. Levine (2003) Diversity, discrimination and
Gustafson, D., E. Rothenberg, K. Blennow, et al. (2003) An 18-year
performance. IIRUPS Working Paper 091–03. Berkeley, CA:
follow-up of overweight and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Intern
Institute of Industrial Relations, University of California, Berkeley.
Lesgold, A. (1984) Acquiring expertise. In: J. Anderson and S.
Hægeland, T. and T. J. Klette (1999) Do higher wages reflect higher
Kosslyn (eds.) Tutorials in Learning and Memory: Essays in Honor
productivity? Education, gender and experience premiums in a
of Gordon Bower. New York: Freeman, pp. 31–60.
matched plant-worker data set. In: J. L. Haltiwanger, J. R. Spletzer,
Levy, B. R. (2003) Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of
J. Theeuwes, and K. Troske (eds.) The Creation and Analysis of
aging self-stereotypes. Journal of Gerontology: PsychologicalEmployer-Employee Matched Data. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp. Sciences, 58B(4), pp. 203–211.
Lindh, T. and B. Malmberg (1999) Age structure and growth in the
Haltivanger, J. C., J. I. Lane, and J. R. Spletzer (1999) Productivity
OECD. Journal of Population Economics, 12(3), pp. 431–449.
differences across employers. The roles of employer size, age and
Lindley, R., et al. (2006) Ageing and Employment: Identification of
human capital. American Economic Review, 89 (2), pp. 94–98. Good Practice to Increase Job Opportunities and Maintain Older
Hamilton, B. H., J. A. Nickerson, and H. Owan (2004) Diversity andWorkers in Employment. Report for the European Commission. Productivity in Production Teams. St. Louis, Missouri: John M. Olin
Brussels: European Commission, Directorate-General for
School of Business, Washington University. Draft, May.
Employment, Social Affairs and Equal Opportunities, Warwick IER.
Hellerstein, J. and D. Neumark (2004) Production function and wage
Lutz, W., A. Goujon, S. K.C., and W. Sanderson (2007)
equation estimation with heterogeneous labor: Evidence from a new
Reconstruction of populations by age, sex and level of educational
matched employer-employee data set. NBER Working Paper 10325.
attainment for 120 countries for 1970–2000. In: The Vienna
Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. Yearbook of Population Research 2007. Vienna: Verlag der
Hellerstein, J. K. and D. Neumark (1995) Are earnings profiles steeper
Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften (forthcoming).
than productivity profiles? Evidence from Israeli firm-level data.
Maitland, S. B., R. C. Intrieri, K. W. Schaie, and S. L. Willis (2000)
Journal of Human Resources, XXX (1), pp. 89–112.
Gender differences and changes in cognitive abilities across the adult
Hellerstein, J. K., D. Neumark, and K. R. Troske (1999) Wages,
life span. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7(1), pp. 32–53.
productivity and worker characteristics: Evidence from plant level
Malmberg, B. and T. Lindh (2002) Population change and economic
production function and wage equations. Journal of Labor
growth in the western world, 1850–1990. Paper presented at the 3rd
Economics, 17, pp. 409–446.
Max Planck Institute Conference on Demographic Macroeconomic
Heywood, J., L. Ho, and X. Wei (1999) The determinants of hiring
Modeling, held in Rostock, Germany, September 2002.
older workers: Evidence from Hong Kong. Industrial and Labour
Manton, K. G., L. Corder, and E. Stallard (1997) Chronic disability
Relations Review, 52, pp. 444–459.
trends in elderly United States populations: 1982–1994. Proceedings
Hirsch, B. T., D. MacPherson, and M. Hardy (2000) Occupational age
of the National Academy of Science, 94, pp. 2593–2598.
structure and access for older workers. Industrial and Labour
Mazzeo, R.S. (2000) Current comment: Exercise and the older adult. Relations Review, 53, pp. 401–418. American College of Sports Medicine. Available at ACSM on the
Horn, J. L. and R. Cattell (1966) Refinement and test of the theory of
fluid and crystallized intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology
www.acsm.org/health%2Bfitness/pdf/currentcomments/eoa.pdf
McDowell, J. (1982) Obsolence of knowledge and career publication
Hujer, R, and D. Radic (2005) Age and skill biased technological
profiles: Some evidence of differences among fields in costs of
change: A multiple treatment approach using a linked employer
interrupted careers. American Economic Review, 72(4), pp. 752–768.
McEvoy, G. M. and W. F. Cascio (1989) Cumulative evidence of the
doku.iab.de/veranstaltungen/2006/CAFE_2006_K1_Radic.pdf
relationship between employee age and job performance. Journal of
Hutchens, R. (1989) Seniority, wages and productivity: A turbulent
Applied Psychology, 74(1), pp. 11–17.
decade. Journal of Economic Perspectives 3(4), pp. 49–64.
Melchior, M., L. F. Berkman, I. Niedhammer, M. Chea, and M.
Ichino, A., G. Schwerdt, R. Winter-Ebmer, and J. Zweimüller (2006) Too
Goldberg (2003) Social relations and self-reported health: A
Old to Work, Too Young to Retire? Paper presented at the Conference of
prospective analysis of the French Gazel cohort. Social Science and
European Association of Labour Economists. Available at:
Medicine, 56, pp. 1817–1830.
http://www.eale.nl/conference2006/Papers%20Friday%2017.00%20–%
Miller, G. F. (1999) Sexual selection for cultural display. In: R.
Dunbar, C. Knight, and C. Power (eds.) The Evolution of Culture:
Ilmakunnas, P., M. Maliranta, and J. Vainiomäki (2004) The role of
An Interdisciplinary View. New Brunswick: Rutgers University
employer and employee characteristics for plant productivity. Journalof Productivity Analysis, 21, pp. 249–276.
Minois, N. and E. Le Bourg (1997) Hypergravity and aging in
Jenkins, A. (2001) Companies’ use of psychometric testing and the
drosophila melonagaster. Conditioned suppression and habituation of
changing demand for skills: A review of the literature. Working
the proboscis extension response. Aging, Experimental and Clinical
Paper. London: Centre for the Economics of Education, London
Munnell, A., S. Sass, and M. Soto (2006) Employers attitudes towards
Johnson, P. (2003) Age, gender and wage in Britain 1830–1930. In:
older workers: Survey results. Series 3, July. Boston College,
Scholliers, P. and L. Schwarz (eds.) Experiencing Wages: Social and
Massachusetts: Center for Retirement Research. Cultural Aspects of Wage Forms in Europe since 1500. New York:
Myerson, J., S. Hale, D. Wagstaff, L. W. Ponn, and G. A. Smith
(1990) The information loss model: A mathematical theory of age-
Jones, B. F. (2005) Age and great invention. NBER Working
related slowing. Psychological Review, 97, pp. 475–486. AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
Neisser, U. (1997) Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests. American
Schmidt, F. L. and J. E. Hunter (1998) The validity and utility of
selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical
Nilsson, S. E., B. Johansson, S. Takkinen, S. Berg, S. Zarit, G.
implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin,
McClearn, and A. Melander (2003) Does aspirin protect against
Alzheimer’s dementia? A study in a Swedish population-based
Schmier, J. K., M. L. Jones, and M. T. Halpern (2006) Cost of obesity in
sample aged =80 years. European Journal of Clinical
the workplace. Scand J Work Environ Health, 32 (1), pp. 5–11. Pharmacology, 59(4), pp. 313–319.
Schneider, L. (2006) Sind ältere beschäftigte weniger produktiv? Eine
Nishimura, K. G., K. Minetaki, M. Shirai, and F. Kurokawa (2002)
empirische analyse anhand des LIAB. [Are older workers less produc-
Effects of information technology and aging work force on labor
tive? An analysis based on the LIAB]. Halle, Germany: Institut für
demand and technological progress in Japanese industries:
Wirtschaftsforschung Halle (IWH), Working Paper Nr. 13.
1980–1998. Discussion Paper CIRJE-F-145. Tokyo: Faculty of
Schoeni, R. F., V. A. Freedman, and R. B. Wallace (2001) Persistent,
consistent, widespread, and robust? Another look at recent trends in
OECD (1999) Thematic Review of the Transition from Initial Education
old-age disability. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 56B, pp. to Working Life. Final Comparative Report, DEELA/ED (99) 11.
Paris: Directorate for Education, Employment, Labour and Social
Schooler, C., M. S. Mulatu, and G. Oates (1999) The continuing effects
Affairs, Education Committee, Organisation for Economic Co-
of substantively complex work in the intellectual functioning of older
workers. Psychology and Aging, 14(3), pp. 483–506.
OECD (2006) Live longer, work longer: A synthesis report. Available
Schwartzman, A. E., D. Gold, D. Andres, T. Y. Arbuckle, and J.
online at www.oecd.org/document/42/0,3343,en_2649_34747
Chaikelson (1987) Stability of intelligence. A 40 year follow-up. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, pp. 244–256.
Osler, W. (1906) The Fixed Period. (3rd chapter in Aequanimitas). New
Scott, F., M. Berger, and J. Garen (1995) Do health insurance and
pension costs reduce the job opportunities of older workers?
Oster, S. and D. S. Hamermesh (1998) Aging and productivity among econ-
Industrial and Labour Relations Review, 48, pp. 775–791.
omists. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 80(1), pp. 154–156.
Simonton, D. K. (1988) Age and outstanding achievement: What do we
Park, D. C., R. E. Nisbett, and T. Hedden (1999) Culture, cognition,
know after a century of research? Psychological Bulletin, 104, pp.
and aging. Journal of Gerontology, 54B, pp. 75–84.
Parkin, T. (2003) Old Age in the Roman world. A cultural and social
Skirbekk, V. (2008) Productivity potential during the life cycle: The
history. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
changing importance of age-specific abilities. Population and
Phelps, R. and J. Shanteau (1978) Livestock judges: How much
Development Review, 34, pp. 191–207.
information can an expert use? Organizational Behavior and Human
Smith, A. D. (1996) Memory. In: J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (eds.)
Performance, 21, pp. 209–219. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. New York: Academic Press,
Prskawetz, A., B. Mahlberg, and V. Skirbekk (Forthcoming 2007) Firm
productivity, workforce age and educational structure in Austrian indus-
Solem, P. (2006) Age and context in subjective work ability changes.
tries. In A. Mason and R. Clark (eds.) Intergenerational Transfers andthe Macroeconomy. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
Solfrizzi, V., F. Panza, and F. Capurso (2003) The role of diet in cogni-
Prskawetz, A., B. Mahlberg, V. Skirbekk, I. Freund, M. Winkler-Dworak,
tive decline. Journal of Neural Transmission, 110(1), pp. 95–110.
T. Lindh, B. Malmberg, A.-C. Jans, O. S. Nordström, and F. Andersson
Spitz-Oener, A. (2006) Technical change, job tasks, and rising
(2005) The impact of population ageing on innovation and productivity
educational demands: Looking outside the wage structure. Journal ofgrowth in Europe. Brussels: European Commission, European
Labor Economics, 24(2), pp. 235–270.
Observatory on Demography and the Social Situation (SSO), Studies on
Tang, J. and C. MacLeod (2006) Labour force ageing and productivity
policy implications of demographic changes in the enlarged EU.
performance in Canada. Canadian Journal of Economics, 39(2), pp.
Raskin, E. (1936) Comparison of scientific and literary ability: A
biographical study of eminent scientists and letters of the nineteenth
Tuddenham, R. D. (1948) Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II.
century. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31, pp. 20–35. American Psychologist, 3, pp. 54–56.
Remery, C. K., J. Henkens, J. Schippers, and P. Ekamper (2003) Managing
U.S. Department of Labor (1957) Comparative job performance by age.
an aging workforce and a tight labor market: Views held by Dutch
Large plants in the men’s footwear and household furniture industries.
employers. Population Research and Policy Review, 22, pp. 21–40. Bulletin No. 1223. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Labor.
Riphahn, R. T. and P. Trübswetter (2006) Population Aging and
van Ours, J., P. Ilmakunnas, V. Skirbekk, and M. Weiss (2007) Aging
and Productivity. Paper presented at the IX Conference Fondazione
Romeu Gordo, L. (2005) Compression of Morbidity and the Labor
Supply of Older People. Working Paper. Berlin: Max Planck
Verhaegen, P. and T. A. Salthouse (1997) Meta-analyses of age-
cognition relations in adulthood. Estimates of linear and nonlinear
Rubin, D. K. and J. M. Perloff (1993) Who works for piece rates and why.
age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), pp. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 627, pp. 1036–1043.
Rybash, J. M., W. Hoyer, and P. A. Roodin (1986) Adult Cognition
Waldman, D. A. and B. J. Avolio (1986) A meta-analysis of age
and Ageing. New York: Pergamon Press.
differences in job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
Salthouse, T. (1984) Effects of age and skills in typing. Journal ofExperimental Psychology, 113, pp. 345–371.
Warr, P. (1994) Age and employment. In: H. Triandis, M. Dunnette,
Salthouse, T. and T. J. Maurer (1996) Aging, job performance and
and L. Hough (eds.) Handbook of Industrial and Organizational
career development. In: J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (eds.)
Psychology. Thousand Oaks, California: Consulting Psychologist
Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. New York: Academic Press,
Zwick, T. (2002) Continuous Training and Firm Productivity in
Schaie, K. W. and S. L. Willis (1986) Can decline in intellectual func-
Germany. ZEW Discussion Paper No. 02–50. Mannheim, Germany:
tioning be reversed? Developmental Psychology, 22(2), pp. 223–232.
Centre for European Economic Research. CorrespondenceWorld Population Program, International Institute for Applied Systems Analysis (IIASA), Schlossplatz 1, A-2361 Laxenburg, AustriaEmail: [email protected]AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING Annex: Overview of employer-employee data sets Sample size Control variables Productivity Age-productivity measurement
Productivity peaks between Manufacturing and
capital and input factorsR&D spending
Gender, race, occupation, Firm’s output or value
AGEING HORIZONS Issue No 8 OXFORD INSTITUTE OF AGEING
TICK DISEASE This is an extensive topic of great importance to Greyhound adopters. The limited space here will serve as a summary of tick disease issues and hopefully a guide for you to pursue further information. WHAT ARE THEY? These are a group of diseases caused by microorganisms transmitted by an attached tick. Racing Greyhounds seem to be disproportionately over represented among
MARKET INFO FOR THE WEEK – Monday 18 January 2010 Table of Contents 1. SA Dividends equities paying out dividends and their last trading day. 2. IdealCFDs Events Calendar Dates for all upcoming Seminars / Profit Centers and Trading 3. Company Calendar 4. SA Publications 5. Foreign Events for the week Summary of daily foreign announcements for the week.